होम
Journal of Architectural Education Search/Research || The Clerk and the Ignoramus
Search/Research || The Clerk and the Ignoramus
Chris Arnoldयह पुस्तक आपको कितनी अच्छी लगी?
फ़ाइल की गुणवत्ता क्या है?
पुस्तक की गुणवत्ता का मूल्यांकन करने के लिए यह पुस्तक डाउनलोड करें
डाउनलोड की गई फ़ाइलों की गुणवत्ता क्या है?
खंड:
32
भाषा:
english
पत्रिका:
JAE
DOI:
10.2307/1424373
Date:
May, 1979
फ़ाइल:
PDF, 746 KB
आपके टैग:
फ़ाइल 1-5 मिनट के भीतर आपके ईमेल पते पर भेजी जाएगी.
फ़ाइल 1-5 मिनट के भीतर आपकी Kindle पर डिलीवर हो जाएगी.
टिप्पणी: आप जो भी पुस्तक अपने Kindle पर भेजना चाहें इसे सत्यापित करना होगा. Amazon Kindle Support से सत्यापन ईमेल के लिए अपना मेलबॉक्स देखें.
टिप्पणी: आप जो भी पुस्तक अपने Kindle पर भेजना चाहें इसे सत्यापित करना होगा. Amazon Kindle Support से सत्यापन ईमेल के लिए अपना मेलबॉक्स देखें.
Conversion to is in progress
Conversion to is failed
0 comments
आप पुस्तक समीक्षा लिख सकते हैं और अपना अनुभव साझा कर सकते हैं. पढ़ूी हुई पुस्तकों के बारे में आपकी राय जानने में अन्य पाठकों को दिलचस्पी होगी. भले ही आपको किताब पसंद हो या न हो, अगर आप इसके बारे में ईमानदारी से और विस्तार से बताएँगे, तो लोग अपने लिए नई रुचिकर पुस्तकें खोज पाएँगे.
1
|
|
2
|
|
The Clerk and the Ignoramus Author(s): Chris Arnold Source: JAE, Vol. 32, No. 4, Search/Research (May, 1979), pp. 2-3 Published by: Taylor & Francis, Ltd. on behalf of the Association of Collegiate Schools of Architecture, Inc. Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1424373 Accessed: 27-06-2016 09:40 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Taylor & Francis, Ltd., Association of Collegiate Schools of Architecture, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to JAE This content downloaded from 128.197.26.12 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:02 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms Chris Arnold lectures at the Department ofAr- while re-search is backtracking to prove chitecture at Berkeley and is the President ofBuild- the truth of the search. The institutionali- ing Systems Development, Inc, San Francisco. zation of research emphasizes the latter task, rather than the former. So researchers research the research- able-that is to say, those things that lend themselves to classical research. This is amazingly limiting, but it is the rec- ognized basis of research. One of the re- sults of this has been the rise of what I would call clerical research. Clerical work consists of copying documents containing ideas and information developed by others, providing numerical data, organiz- The Clerk and the Ignoramus ing data in ways devised by others, and drawing conclusions through the use of precise criteria set at a higher level. The essence of clericalism is the suspension of judgment and its replacement by automatic meth; ods. This is a description of much research: thought is replaced by the accumulation of data. By some paradoxical S. . well-intentioned efforts to determine by the crudest kind of cost-benefit how to decide planning issues may well be bracketed by our descendants with the arguments of medieval theologians over the number of angels who could dance on the point of a needle-Max Nicholson. Style, the way one performs an activity, is associated with a personal approach that is predominantly aesthetic. Style is qualified as beautiful, elegant, crude: style used as a qualifier on its own implies excellence in execution, rather than content. The recognition of style in performance is itself a form of elitist praise, a term now replaced in sports and show business by the term class. However expressed, there seems to be a need for this recognition of personal excellence and idiosyncrasy. This whole notion seems alien to the idea of classical search, "the accumulation of knowledge by systematic observation, deliberate experiment, rational theory."') Intrinsic to this are the concepts of testing and measurement: the setting of criteria, testing to establislh by dispassionate measurement whether these are met, and evaluation of results to determine if an hypothesis has been proven. But scientists agree that style is promi- nent in their field. If you doubt the exis- tence of style in research-personal, idiosyncratic, aesthetic-there are books like James Watson's The Double Helix to make clear its importance to researchers working at the very highest level of scien- tific sophistication. It becomes even more apparent in Horace Judson's story of DNA,2 in which the styles of the main participants are brilliantly described. Listen to the physicist Max Perutz: 1 sometimes enviedJim (Watson). My own problem took thousands of hours of hard work, measurements, calculations. I often thought 2 there must be some way to cut through it-that there must be, zif only I could see it, an elegant solution. There wasn't any. ForJim's there was an elegant solution, which is what I admired. He found it partly because he never made the mistake ofconfusing hard work with hard thinking-he has never spent more than two years on any problem. Here are two styles-the "systematic process involving experiment... " is in both-but in Watson's case, it is concealed quirk, the clerical interpretation of numerical data is then hailed as the measure of truth, while insight and judgment are deemed unscientific and suspect. We have slipped into a situation in which the value of much of our research is gauged by the degree to which it is methodologi- by a scientific attitude operating at a totally cally consistent and internally elegant, not by the usefulness of the hypothesis that it tion of scientific research. exemplary research is done to authenticate different plane of thought, involvement and order than is expressed in our defini- Style begins as an individual way of working, but at some point there is a transfer to the work itself, which establishes its own style, to which later workers conform. sets out to prove. Much elegant and foolish and useless hypotheses. Hypotheses, the flywheels of the research process, are arrived at through a creative route which is highly personal, unpredictable, uncontrollable, and some- We are familiar with this phenomenon in architecture, for its history is predominantly one of style. We can see-without really understanding the mechanism-that Wegener, who, as a lecturer on meteorology and astronomy in Germany in 1910, the individual way of working of a Mies van was struck with a strange idea to which he der Rohe or Le Corbusier becomes a style devoted his life. His hypothesis, which he published extensively, was that the geographical fit between South America and which then imposes its own discipline on a generation of followers. Research at the double helix level is ob- times cranky. Take the case of Alfred Africa suggested that these continents had once been one, and had then split and viously not the norm-it is an aspiration. As such, it seems that research should be drifted apart. The other continents could done in different ways, and use such qual- more or less be fitted together in the same ities as insight, intuition, imagination, boldness, risk-taking-qualities that seem to me to be intrinsic to the pursuit of knowledge. But these qualities and ways of working are suspect to the conventional researcher, whose tools are restricted and laborious. There is a reason for this: the mass of today's research is not directed towards the pursuit of knowledge, but towards its au- thentication. Science tells us that a theory or observation is not part of knowledge until proven, and so research is often not the noble investigation of unknown terri- sort of pattern. He argued persuasively and his theory of continental drift was widely debated in the 1920s, but it was soon shown that his data were hopelessly inaccurate and the dynamic forces too small by a factor of a million or so. As one of his opponents said, "it is not scientific, but takes the familiar course of an initial idea, a selective search through the literature for corroborative evidence, ignoring most of the facts that are opposed to the idea, ending in a state of auto-intoxication in which the subjective idea comes to be considered on objective fact." After tory, but its measurement after someone Wegener's death, his theory gradually was has taken the risk to discover it. Thus there dropped from the teaching and textbooks are two distinct styles of researching: one of geology. long been recognized-by, for example, considered to be perfectly correct.4 The, story makes an important point: we relating to discovery (or invention), and the other to authentication. They have the French mathematician, Poincare: "it is by logic that we prove, but by intuition that we discover."3 The discovery is search, The theory of continental drift is now have always known that cranks are not to be trusted, but the elaborate safeguards of authentication can also be faulty, and proof This content downloaded from 128.197.26.12 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:02 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms is not absolute, but limited by the state of must, as the practicing designer does, be knowledge. Uncertainty is uncomfortable, prepared to accept that the search is much more important than the research, and that the question of hypothesis is more important that clerical authentication. Parts of but healthy-it is the cocksureness of much conventional research and technol- ogy that is the most irritating and damaging. I feel that our uncertainties about the environment are much healthier than the the design can be proven: the laws of physics and biology are immutable and the certainties "proven" by our clerical pro- 7,., .i:. . t l, ,.." -, rc' cesses. Our current obsession with authentication in research can lead us into some foolish situations. Public recognition of a problem, followed by political recognition, enables massive research efforts to begin, generally on very conservative and methodologically unassailable lines. Scien- \3 tific research of this type may, of course, I play a role in revealing aspects of a problem of which the public would only much later, or never, become aware. Massive research into carcinogenics, for example, can reveal dangers that less sophisticated observation would probably never uncover. But this same process also reveals "dangers" that the public often, and probably rightly, regards as nonsense. The 1958 Delaney Amendment to the Food, Drug and Cosmetics Act bans any food additive shown to cause cancer in any animal test, so the FDA had to ban saccharin when tests showed that rats became cancerous after consuming the equivalent of humans consuming 800 cans of diet soda a day for life. This represents a triumph of clerical scien- . ' : I g / /' ! : Aalto's sketch solution to a daylighting problem. resides in style. In earlier times, the great scientist was an all-rounder: he combined his imaginative flashes with physical experimentation and analysis that led to proof. This is still sometimes true-it is clearly true in the case of the double helix, whose discovery was extraordinarily similar to the design process in architecture. Essentially, the double helix riddle was solved by building a model that met certain experimentally tific thinking over reasonable judgment. observed criteria. In our language, the agency agreement has set limits on the ex- know this feeling: the point at which a de- Today (February 1979), a new intertent to which animal experiments involving gross overdoses should be accepted. Sometimes style, in the sense of person- ality, can dominate the traditional safeguards that the classical authentication process is designed to guard against. Such was the case of Sir Cyril Burt, English psy- chologist, whose work had an enormous, worldwide influence, and who, as educational psychologist to the London County Council, also had access to monumental quantities of research data. Most of his energy and research went into proving that mental abilities are inborn, easily measured, and concentrated in higher social classes of the white community. Critical to the authentication of this viewpoint was a paper, Intelligence and Social Mobility, purportedly based on the records of 40,000 London families. It has recently been shown, conclusively, that his research was fraudulent---even to the extent that he used and quoted imaginary research assistants. How did he get away with it? One answer lies in his style: he was a formidable personality, with a deadly facility for criticizing others' work, and an immense skill with language. He could "tear to ribbons anything shoddy or inconsistent." When he died in 1972, his obituary said "his sharp intellect and vast erudition... leave a total impression of immense quality, of a born nobleman."s We have more modern words now for his type; but the important thing is the immense power that model met the program. Most designers sign answers the demands of the program and solves the formal puzzle. Conven- tional architectural design meets many of the conditions for discovery: every designer develops an hypothesis about how the program is to be met; experiments are performed in drawings and models, even in simple, analytical calculations. Alvar Aalto's 1927 drawing is a beautiful example of designer's research. It shows a visual exploration of a problem whose solution will be a concrete realization of gra- phics-an appropriate research tool for the problem. The building itself is the final experiment-its occupancy is the test. Missing in design must be tuned to these. But design as a whole must also involve value judg- ment and risk. I do not draw the currently popular dis- tinction between research and design. On the contrary, I believe that it is only when design is seen as a conscious research activity, and vice versa, that significant breakthroughs can be made. It is nonsense to believe that a group of mindless form-givers are fed absolute knowledge by a group of logical analysts, through some kind of filter, and to improve design, one only has to improve the filter and improve the analysis. It is symptomatic of current attitudes that Progressive Architecture, in attempting to define research for purposes of its awards program, defined research as that which is not design. I think that the poor quality of most environmental research, and the general insecurity and gloom of many of its practitioners, stems from the subliminal realization that the real research is going on in design offices. I am not suggesting the complete renunciation of classical research in favor of individual whims and intuitions-a world of poets or ofclerks is equally alarming. But I would suggest that environmental design is still groping for its appropriate style, and the designer's style is a more fruitful model for environmental research than the pro- cess of authentication. The testing and evaluation of design-style research will remain to some extent unsusceptible to numerical objective evaluation--qualita- tive and appropriate only within a particular cultural context. As such, we cannot afford to limit research to value-free evaluation, because the environment is predominantly a qualitative and value-rich field. A "value-free" environmental design re- search will also tend to be valueless. References this process, in terms of the requirements 'Ziman, John; The Force of Knowledge, Cambridge, 1976. But the architectural world is very rich in DNA," The New Yorker, November 27, December 4, 11, 1978. of the classical model, are the objective criteria for testing, and their evaluation. hypotheses and experiments, and I believe it is true that the kinds of objective and measurable criteria that are the basis of the natural and physical sciences do not exist in the combined physical, social, cultural, and political context of the environmental world. I seriously question whether they ever will, and I believe the search for objective criteria that can bring environmen- 2Judson, Horace Freeland; "Annals of Science: 3Poincare, quoted by biophysicist Thomas Hayes, in the Lawrence Berkeley Laboratory News Magazine, Summer 1977, 4This account is adaptedfrom the The Force of Knowledge. SGillie, Oliver; "Sir Cyril Burt and the Great IQ Fraud," The New Statesman, London, November 24, 1978. !Judson: op cit. 'Aalto drawing from Alvar Aalto, Wittenborn & tal research into line with the "proving"i tradition of classical science is abortive, Company, New York, 1963. and can lead only to insecurity and frustra- The heading quotation by Max Nicholson is from We must be prepared to accept a much broader basis of authentication, and we Department of the Environment, Her Majesty's Stationery Office, London, 1972. tion. How Do You Want To Live, published by the This content downloaded from 128.197.26.12 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:02 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 3